The global aim of the proposal is to expand our knowledge on the functioning of the fronto-striatal system and the relation to behavioral rigidity, and how these are affected by dopaminergic modulation. The specific aim is to study the effects of…
ID
Source
Brief title
Condition
- Other condition
- Psychiatric disorders NEC
Synonym
Health condition
Autisme spectrum stoornissen
Research involving
Sponsors and support
Intervention
Outcome measures
Primary outcome
The Error-Related negativity (ERN) is a peak in the ERP signal measured
directly after the participant makes a mistake; in the case of the Go/ NoGo
task, this means that the ERN is measured when the participant inadvertently
presses the response button with a NoGo trial. The ERN is a measure of
self-monitoring, which is hypothesized to be low in children with ASD with
rigid behavior at baseline. The use of risperidone enhances cingulate cortex
activity, resulting in larger ERN amplitudes. Response times, and particularly
the delay in reaction time after making a mistake, are also measured. Usually,
the reaction times after an erronous response are slower; we expect that this
post-error slowing at baseline is relatively small in children with ASD, but
that it increases due to the use of risperidone. Both effect measures will be
correlated with the degree of rigid behavior in the children with ASD, here we
expect stronger effects with higher levels of rigid behavior at baseline.
Secondary outcome
Source localizations of the ERN are applied, meaning that it is measured
whether the ERN can actually be located in the anterior cingulate cortex. This
source localization is done by using Brain Electrical Source Analysis software.
Previous studies have shown that using 64 electrodes, as it is the case in our
study, will provide robust signals that are appropriate for measuring ERN
source localization (Vlamings et al, 2008).
Background summary
Behavioral rigidity is an important restricting factor in the development of
adaptive social functioning. However, it is also a characteristic of early
childhood; very young children, beginning in the first year of life, increasing
around three years of age and typically declining after age four, display
ritualistic and rigid behaviors in daily life, such as ordering and arranging
objects in circumscribed ways or insisting on specific bedtime rituals.
Although in typical development this behavioral rigidity gradually declines, it
remains a significant and exaggerated characteristic in individuals with autism
spectrum disorders (ASD).
Both in human and animal research, dopaminergically innervated interactions in
the fronto-striatal brain network have been implicated in rigid and stereotypic
behavior, with a prominent role for the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC).
Indeed, there is recent evidence for a relation between clinical measures of
behavioral rigidity in ASD and the fronto-striatal system. E.g. it was shown
that impairments in fronto-striatal white matter tracts are present in
individuals with ASD which relate to their clinical rigidity (Thakkar et al.,
2008). Also, a recent study showed a relation between clinically defined
rigidity in ASD, formal measures of rigid and stereotypic behavior, i.e. set
shifting abilities in neuropsychological tests, and ACC functioning (Shafritz
et al., 2008).
Functioning of the fronto-striatal network can be studied in humans by
measuring the event-related brain response to errors, the so-called
error-related negativity (ERN). The origin of ERN activity has been repeatedly
localized within the ACC (Dehaene et al., 1994). It is thought that ERN
activity reflects self monitoring, a prerequisite to flexible and adaptive
behavior, and it is therefore conceptually related to behavioral rigidity. We
have shown atypical ERN activity in children with ASD (Vlamings et al., 2008).
There is evidence that the fronto-striatal system shows a protracted
development in healthy children, as indicated by changes in childhood in ACC
functioning, but it is not known whether rigid and stereotypic behavior in
individuals with ASD is qualitatively different from that seen in typically
developing children, and how this relates to the abnormal development of the
fronto-striatal system.
Developmental changes in ERN activity indicate an increase in ACC activity and
therefore maturity of the DA innervated connections (Segalowitz et al., 2009).
The putative role of dopamine in behavioral rigidity is further suggested by
the fact that in clinical practice, the atypical antipsychotic risperidone is
often used to treat individuals with ASD, as it ameliorates the rigid and
repetitive behavior associated with this disorder. Its mode of action, however,
is not clear. We hypothesize that risperidone enhances activity of the
ACC-dopamine midbrain network, as reflected in the ERN, and that this is
directly related to clinically relevant measures of rigidity. In this study we
want to find evidence for this hypothesis.
Study objective
The global aim of the proposal is to expand our knowledge on the functioning of
the fronto-striatal system and the relation to behavioral rigidity, and how
these are affected by dopaminergic modulation. The specific aim is to study the
effects of risperidone on fronto-striatal functioning in individuals with ASD,
and the effects on behavioral rigidity. We hypothesize that risperidone
enhances activity of the fronto-striatal network, as reflected in the ERN, and
that this is directly related to improvement on clinical measures of behavioral
rigidity.
Study design
Participants will conduct a go/nogo task twice, the second one month after the
first administration. They will conduct the task just before medication is
started and one month after. During the task we will measure event-related
brain potentials (ERPs) that will be analyzed with respect to latency and
amplitude of the ERN, as well as to source localization. In addition, we will
quantify rigidity in daily life. This will be done with diagnostic information
obtained from the ADI-R.
A go/nogo task will be used (Durston et al., 2002). In this task the children
will be asked to press a button in response to visually presented stimuli (go
trials), but to avoid responding to rare non-targets (nogo trials). They will
perform 5 blocks, each containing 108 stimulus presentations, with 75% go
trials, resulting in a total of 135 nogo trials. Additionally, we vary the
number of go trials preceding a nogo trial, with 1, 3, or 5 preceding go
trials. Each type of nogo trial therefore contains 45 stimulus presentations.
To make the study better suitable for use with children, stimuli will be taken
from the Pokemon cartoon series. Stimulus duration is 500 ms and the intertrial
interval will vary randomly between 500 - 1500 ms, during which a central
fixation stimulus is presented on screen. The children are asked to push a
designated button every time a go trial occurs, but to withhold from responding
as soon as the character 'Meowth' appears on screen.
Many studies using a go/nogo paradigm have found ACC activity following an
erronous nogo-response, which makes this task ideally suited for our study
(e.g. Hester et al., 2004).
EEGs will be recorded at a sample rate of 2048 Hz from 64 locations using
standard Ag/AgCl pin-type active electrodes (BIOSEMI, Amsterdam, the
Netherlands) mounted in an elastic cap, referenced to two additional electrodes
(Common Mode Sense, and Driven Right Leg) during recording. The children are
seated in a comfortable chair in front of a computer screen in an electrically
shielded room. After a standardized instruction the children perform a short
practice block consisting of 24 trials. After application of the electrodes the
children perform the 5 experimental blocks (each lasting less than 3 min).
Study burden and risks
The EEG itself does not elicit any risk of complications. The burden for the
participant is minimal as it only requires administration of two EEGs during
planned visits to the clinic. Travel expenses will be reimbursed, and all
participants will receive a 10 euro cheque after each visit to the UMC Utrecht.
The benefits are twofold. First, the study will provide clinicians with
neurobiological markers for behavioral rigidity related to ASD. Although
repetitive behaviors are included in all major diagnostic criteria for ASD and
can be reliably diagnosed in children, it is unclear, as yet, whether this
relates to the abnormal development of ACC functioning and maturation of the
dopaminergic neuronal connections between ACC and the limbic system. Second,
the study is expected to provide clinicians with more knowledge about
underlying mechanisms leading to successful treatment of rigid and stereotypic
behavior. The daily clinical practice clearly requires such knowledge, as it is
unknown beforehand who will respond positively to risperidone treatment and who
will not.
Heidelberglaan 100
3584 CX
NL
Heidelberglaan 100
3584 CX
NL
Listed location countries
Age
Inclusion criteria
Children aged 6 to 12, who are diagnosed with ASD at the department of child psychiatry. All children who will be prescribed risperidone for stereotypic behavior, but who are medication-naive at the beginning of the study, will be asked to participate. Based on the known male: female ratio of about 4:1 (Fombonne, 2002), we expect to include around 20 boys and 5 girls in our study. Diagnostic criteria are according to the DSM-IV (APA, 1994), and verified by the ADOS (Lord et al., 1989) and ADI-R (Lord et al., 1994). IQs of all participants will be measured by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Dutch edition (WISC-III-NL).
Exclusion criteria
Individuals who are either not medication-naive at baseline, with a known brain dysfunction, total IQ score below 75 or with psychiatric pathology other than ASD will be excluded.
Design
Recruitment
Followed up by the following (possibly more current) registration
No registrations found.
Other (possibly less up-to-date) registrations in this register
No registrations found.
In other registers
Register | ID |
---|---|
CCMO | NL33261.041.10 |